Psychologist Bangkok Logo
Schedule: Monday to Sunday: 11am - 6pm
admin@psychologistbangkok.com+66 (0) 922887234
hello world!
Published: November 19, 2020
Eric Mason

A Study On The Influence Of Thai Cultural Factors On Attitudes and Perceptions of ADHD

Abstract: The current research examined Thai cultural factors, such as religiosity, stigmatization of mental disorders, holistic thinking, and health locus of control towards attitudes and perceptions of ADHD, moderated by prior exposure to ADHD. The Thai translated scales Multidimensional Health Locus of Control Scales (MHLC), the SBI (Religious Beliefs/Practices), the Community Attitudes towards the Mentally Ill scale, the ADHD Stigma questionnaire (ASQ) (attitudes towards ADHD), Choi’s Analysis-Holism Scale, the ADHD Beliefs Scale-revised (perceptions of ADHD) were used for testing the model. The factor structure was examined via confirmatory factor analysis and reliability and validity were established initially before testing the model. Further, SEM was utilized to test the relationships between the aforementioned cultural factors and attitudes and perceptions of ADHD (n = 323). The results indicated significant relationships between cultural factors and negative opinions of ADHD. The current research aimed to contribute to the understanding of unique aspects of ADHD in Thailand. Future research may explore methods to increase knowledge of ADHD and reduce negative attitudes/perceptions of ADHD.

Keywords: ADHD, Thailand, Culture, Stigma, Exposure to ADHD, Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Introduction. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a complex disorder which presents with both subtle and obvious symptoms. Due to its subtleties, culture may heavily influence both diagnostic rates and treatment rates of ADHD. Research has demonstrated that the prevalence of ADHD varies across cultures. For example, rates of ADHD in France tend to be lower when compared to rates in Germany. Despite proximity and cultural similarities, ADHD rates remain very different between these two countries. This is not an isolated example. Rates of ADHD are different across practically all countries. This points towards the influence that culture or cultural perception has on rates of ADHD. Furthermore, knowledge and awareness of ADHD also influences perceptions of the disorder. Indeed, there are multiple ways in which cultural factors may influence ADHD rates—possibly leading to both under or over diagnosis of the disorder (Faraone, Sergeant, Gillberg, & Biederman, 2003).

There is limited research on ADHD in Thailand. According to Visanuyothin, et al. (2012), rates of ADHD in Thailand are 8.1%. However, according to Benjasuwantep, et al. (2002) rates were 6.1%. Another study found ADHD rates in Thailand to be much lower, overall, at 2.2% (Sakboonyarat, 2018). ADHD, Inattentive was reported as the higher by one study at 3.8%, while ADHD Hyperactive/Impulsive type were the lowest (Visanuyothin et. al, 2013). Rates for ADHD, Hyperactive/Impulsive type being lower would be in line with Barkly’s (2001) assertion that the number of reported cases of ADHD in Thailand are lower due to cultural factors which train children to speak quietly in public and encourage obedience to authority figures. As a result, as mentioned above, ADHD may be more likely to present Inattentive type, which was also supported by Visanuyothin, et al. (2013).

Aspects of certain cultures may, in fact, lead to lower rates of ADHD or behaviors associated with ADHD. However, it is more likely that culture has a greater effect on one’s perceptions and attitudes towards ADHD. For example, some cultures may find certain behaviors in children to be pathological—leading to a greater likelihood for a diagnosis of ADHD, while other cultures may be more tolerant or find such behaviors age appropriate or normal. Such differences in cultural perceptions of ADHD would certainly lead to differing rates of ADHD between different cultures (Reid et al., 1998).

Indeed, cultural factors play an important role in certain aspects of how the public views ADHD. Cultural factors prevalent in Thailand that may influence perceptions and attitudes towards ADHD have not been well researched in Thailand. The current research explored cultural factors that influence perceptions and attitudes towards ADHD, such as tendency to stigmatize mental disorders, holistic thinking, religiosity, and health locus of control (Sakboonyarat, 2018).

Religiosity. Thailand scores very high in religiosity with 94% identifying as Buddhist (Taylor, 2008). Previous research in the United States, reported that those high in religiosity held negative opinions towards ADHD, although the research participants in this study identified as Christians (Li, 2013). Research has found a strong link between religiosity and perceptions/attitudes towards mental illness. Those who report being more religious often have more negative views towards mental illness. What is more, those who are higher in religiosity tend to be less likely to seek out treatment for mental disorders. Indeed, there is a correlation between religiosity and stigmatization of mental disorders. Therefore, it follows that those high in religiosity may stigmatize ADHD, as well.  

Stigmatization. Those with mental disorders are often stigmatized across multiple cultures and societies. People with mental disorders are frequently regarded as dangerous and unpredictable, which creates fear and misunderstanding. Indeed, stigmatization is so pervasive in some cultures, those with mental disorders may even stigmatize themselves (self-stigmatization). Although efforts have been made in Western cultures to reduce stigmatization of mental illness, researchers have found that stigmatization remains higher in developing countries than in developed countries (Lauber & Roessler, 2009). One unique aspect of Thai culture which is related to stigmatization is the cultural phenomena of spirit possession. Being possessed by a malevolent spirit, in turn, leads to those affected (possibly suffering from a mental disorder) to be stigmatized by Thai society (Kaewprom , Curtis, & Deane, 2011). In other words, it is considered to be undesirable, with those being possessed to have been cursed or having brought it on through bad karma (Wong-Anuchit, 2016). Although the researcher is unaware of previous research on stigmatization of ADHD in Thailand, as the literature supports that Thai culture may lead Thais to stigmatize mental disorders, it may be deduced that there would be a link between stigmatization of mental disorders and negative attitudes and perceptions of ADHD.

Locus of Control. The concept of locus of control refers to what extent individuals feel that they are able to influence their environment. Those who believe that they are able to strongly exert influence over their environment are said to have an internal locus of control, while those who feel that they have limited influence over their environment are said to have external locus of control. Those who are from collectivistic cultures, such as Thai culture, tend to have an external locus of control, while those from individualistic cultures usually have an internal locus of control (Cheng & Cheung, 2013). Previous research points out that those with lower internal locus of control may be more likely to have negative views of mental health disorders (Beckman, 1972). ADHD is a mental disorder leading one to extrapolate that those who hold negative views of mental health disorders, would also hold negative views of ADHD.

Holistic Thinking. A holistic thinking style could possibly affect perceptions of mental disorders, including ADHD. Research has found that that those from collectivistic cultures often have a more holistic thinking style, while those from individualistic cultures tend to have a more analytical style of thinking. In general, it can be said that those from Asian countries are more likely to have a holistic style of thinking, since Asian cultures are more often collectivistic.

People from Asian cultures are more likely to take into account the context of the situation when evaluating causation, while Westerners are more likely to focus on the individual in attributing causation (Nisbett & Miyamoto, 2005). Research in Singapore linked to holistic thinking to collective culpability theory and found that Singaporeans were more likely to attribute blame to perceived wrongdoers due to circumstances, rather than view unassociated events or circumstances as distinctly separate from the perceived wrongdoer (Kwan & Chiu, 2014). Therefore, similarly this link between holistic and tendency to inaccurately associate blame on others may result in negative attitudes and perceptions of ADHD in Thailand.

In order to determine how these cultural factors influence perceptions and attitudes towards ADHD, it is necessary to establish the psychometric properties of instruments used to measure these cultural factors that have been developed for Western cultures for use in a Thai context. CFA was used on the following scales: System of Belief Inventor (SBI) (used to measure religiosity), the Community Attitudes towards the Mentally Ill Scale (used to measure stigmatization of mental disorders), Choi’s Analysis-Holism Scale (used to measure holistic thinking), the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control Scale (used to measure locus of control), the ADHD Stigma Questionnaire (used to measure attitudes towards ADHD), and the ADHD Beliefs Scale-revised (used to measure perceptions of ADHD). After CFA, SEM was applied to determine the relationship between the cultural factors and attitudes and perceptions of ADHD.

Figure 1

Path Mode tested via SEM                                                                                                            

Hypothesis. This researcher hypothesized that there would be a link between the cultural factors described above and attitudes towards ADHD and perceptions of ADHD. Specifically, this researcher proposed that the relationships between the cultural factors and attitudes towards ADHD and perceptions of ADHD are as follows:

H1 Religiosity has an influence on attitudes towards ADHD. The higher in religiosity Thai people are, the more negative are their attitudes.

H2 Religiosity has an influence on perceptions of ADHD. The higher in religiosity Thai people are, the more negative are their perceptions.

H3 Stigmatization has an influence on attitudes towards ADHD. The higher in stigmatization towards mental illness Thai people are, the more negative are their     attitudes towards ADHD.

H4 Stigmatization has an influence on perceptions of ADHD. The higher in stigmatization towards mental illness Thai people are, the more negative are their     perceptions of ADHD.

H5 Holistic thinking has an influence on attitudes towards ADHD. The higher in   holistic thinking Thai people are, the more negative were their attitudes towards   ADHD.

H6 Holistic thinking has an influence on perceptions of ADHD. The higher in holistic thinking Thai people are, the more negative are their perceptions of ADHD.

H7 Health locus of control has an influence on attitudes towards ADHD. The lower in internal locus of control, the more negative are attitudes towards ADHD.

H7 Health locus of control has an influence on perceptions of ADHD. The lower in internal locus of control, the more negative are perceptions of ADHD.

Methods

Design. Step 1: Confirmatory factor Analysis (CFA) was conducted to access the adequacy of the factor structure identified via EFA. Unlike EFA, CFA allowed the researcher to posit explicitly one or more a priori models. Step 2:The path model was tested via SEM. Step 3: The moderating effect of previous exposure or no exposure to ADHD was investigated.

ParticipantsSample size was 323 Thai adults. 140 (43%) of participants were male and 183 (57%) were female. 149 (46%) of participants were between 18 and 28 years old; 89 (27.6%) were 29 to 39 years old; 49 (15.1%) were 40 to 50 years old; 35 (11%) were 51 to 75 years old. Regarding highest educational levels completed by participants, 146 (45%) completed high school; 126 (39%) completed a bachelor’s degree; 45 (14%) completed less than high school; 5 (1.5%) completed a graduate degree (Master’s Degree or PhD). Therefore, 277 (86%) of participants had at least a high school education. Participants were recruited primarily in Bangkok, Udon Thani, and Nong Khai. Bangkok is located in central Thailand, while Udon Thani and Nong Khai are in North-East Thailand. Convenience sampling method was employed for data collection.

Table 1

 Demographics Summary, n = 323

Demographics n Educational Level n
Female 183 High School 146
Male 140 BA/BS 126
Non-Bangkok 226 Middle School 45
Bangkok 97 Graduate School  5

Procedure. The data was collected from the participants in the paper form. The instruments were translated to Thai by professional translators and were then back translated to English to ensure there were no discrepancies in translation. The questionnaire comprised of the instruments listed above and consisted of 147 questions. Participants took about 20 minutes on average to complete the questionnaire.

Data Analysis.  Initially Confirmatory factor analysis was done to look at the factor structure of the constructs and convergent and divergent validity were determined. To investigate the influence of religiosity, stigmatization (tendency to stigmatize mental disorders), locus of control, and holistic thinking on attitudes and perceptions of ADHD, the path analysis was employed using SEM. The goodness of fit of posited path model was tested using path analysis.

Results. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) (n = 323) was carried out to evaluate the identified factor structures of the Thai-translated scales of the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control Scale (locus of control) (internality dimension only), the SBI (religiosity), the Community Attitude Towards the Mentally Ill Scale (stigmatization), Choi’s Analysis-Holism Scale (holistic thinking), ADHD Stigma Questionnaire (attitudes towards ADHD), and ADHD Beliefs Scale (perceptions of ADHD). CFA, unlike exploratory factor analysis, allows the researcher to explicitly posit an a priori model (e.g., on the basis of the factors identified in the Western-based original scale) and to assess the fit of this model to the observed data. Figure 2 shows the confirmatory factor analysis. The table 2 presents the standardized regression weights

Table 2

CFA: Standardized Regress Weights, p value, and critical ratio _________________________________________________________________________

Parameters                                             Standard                                p                      Critical

                                                          Regress. Weights                                                Ratio

___________________________________________________________________________

REL1  <—Religiosity (REL)                          0.613                                                  

REL2  <—Religiosity (REL)                         0.970                           ***                  8.749

REL3  <— Religiosity (REL)             0.550                           ***                  8.839

INT1   <—Internal Locus (INT)                     0.823                                                  

INT2   <— Internal Locus (INT)                    0.843                           ***                  11.132

HOL1  <—Holistic (HOL)                              0.914                                                  

HOL2  <—Holistic (HOL)                              0.855                           ***                  23.349

HOL3  <—Holistic (HOL)                              0.741                           ***                  16.570

HOL4  <—Holistic (HOL)                              0.827                           ***                  20.526

STIGM1 <—Stigmatization (STG)                0.591                                                  

STIGM2 <—Stigmatization (STG)                0.733                           ***                  9.214

STIGM3 <—Stigmatization (STG)                0.770                           ***                  9.358

STIGM4 <—Stigmatization (STG)                0.396                           ***                  5.864

ATTP1 <—Attitude ADHD (ATT)                0.855                                                  

ATTP2 <—Attitude ADHD (ATT)                0.897                            ***                  21.041

ATTP3 <—Attitude ADHD (ATT)                0.895                           ***                  20.853

PER1 <—Perception ADHD (PER)               0.690                                                  

PER2 <—Perception ADHD (PER)               0.826                           ***                  12.072

PER3 <—Perception ADHD (PER)               0.756                           ***                  11.489

___________________________________________________________________________

As indicated by table 2 and figure 2, each of the latent constructs had highly significant relationships (p<.001) with each of their respective parcels—indicating that each instrument measures what it proposes to measure (high convergent validity). The standardized regression coefficients (factor loadings) ranged from .591 to .970. The percentage of unexplained variance ranged for the 19 indicator variables ranged from 22% (78% explained) (attitudes towards ADHD) to 59% (41% explained) (stigmatization).

Convergent validity of each of the instruments can be determined by the CFA model by examining if each indicator variable’s standardized loading/coefficient with its underlying latent construct is significant (greater than twice its standard error) (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988). A standardized coefficient is significant if its associated critical ratio is   >  + 1.96. The critical ratio test revealed that the standardized loadings for all 19 indicator variables are statistically significant, which indicates convergent validity for all the instruments.

Discriminant validity was assessed by how strongly each factor correlated with itself as compared to how well each factor correlated with other factors. This indicates that each instrument is able to distinguish the latent construct it measures from the other latent constructs. Table 3 shows that when correlation coefficients for a factor are ranked from highest to lowest it results the highest correlations coefficients lining up on the diagonal, which is representative of discriminate validity across all factors.

Table 3

Discriminant Validity

  REL INT HOL STG ATT PER
REL 0.540 0.031 0.068 0.011 0.020 0.077
INT   0.694 0.260 0.102 0.001 0.212
HOL     0.713 0.000 0.030 0.504
STG       0.409 0.223 0.000
ATT         0.779 0.021
PER           0.577

Path analysis to investigate the relationship of religiosity, locus of control, stigmatization, and holistic thinking on perceptions and attitudes towards ADHD

The hypothesized model was tested to see how well it explained the relationships of religiosity, locus of control, stigmatization, and holistic thinking on participants’ perceptions and attitudes towards ADHD.

Base Model: Structural model between the four independent variables of religiosity, locus of control, stigmatization, holistic thinking and the two dependent variables of attitudes towards ADHD and perceptions of ADHD

SEM was used to test the path model (n = 323). According to the direct model, the four independent variables directly have an association with the two dependent variables. The cultural constructs denoted by the independent variables represent Thai cultural concepts that differ from Western culture. The fit of the path model posited to represent the structural relationships between the independent variables and the dependent variables was evaluated via SEM. This method analyzed the covariance matrix missed generated from the model’s measurement variables.

The overall chi-square goodness-of-fit value was significant, χ2(df = 2) = 259.917, p < .001, and the incremental fit indices (IFI, TLI, CFI) are above .90. This indicates that model is a good fit when compared to a null or independence model in that the posited model represented over a 90% improvement in fit over the null or independence model; therefore, supporting the structure of the posited direct path model. The RMSEA value of 0.057 is also with the acceptable range; thus, indicating that the model fits the population covariance matrix well. In addition, the PNFI value was 0.690 which is used for comparing the goodness-of-fit for competing values. Structural Model (Base Model): Standardized Regression Coefficients. As can be seen in table 4 religiosity, stigmatization, holistic thinking, and internal locus of control were significantly directly associated with the criterion variables of attitudes towards ADHD and perceptions of ADHD.

Table 4

Standardized regression weights, p value, and critical ratio of the structural model

___________________________________________________________________________

Parameters                                                  Standard.                          p                       Critical

                                                                Regress. Weights                                          Ratio

___________________________________________________________________________

Attitude ADHD<—Religiosity                      0.148                   0.009                       2.616

Attitude ADHD<—Internal Locus                0.048                   0.522                       0.640

Attitude ADHD<—Stigmatization                0.467                   ***                          5.823

Attitude ADHD <—Holistic                          0.197                   0.006                       2.769

Perception ADHD <—Internal Locus            0.170                    0.014                      2.470

Perception ADHD <—Stigmatization            0.094                    0.127                      1.527

Perception ADHD<—Holistic                        0.599                    ***                         8.162

Perception ADHD<—Religiosity                  -0.103                    0.044                     -2.016

___________________________________________________________________________

Table 5

Factor Covariances for the Structural Model

___________________________________________________________________________

Parameters                                                Standard                       p                          Critical

                                                             Regress. Weights                                             Ratio

___________________________________________________________________________

Religiosity       <–>     Stigmatization           0.091                   0.173                                    1.364

Religiosity       <–>     Internal Locus          -0.169                    0.008                                    -2.641

Internal Locus <–>     Holistic                      0.513                    ***                           6.988

Stigmatization <–>     Holistic                      0.014                    0.835                                    0.208

Internal Locus <–>     Stigmatization          -0.298                    ***                           -3.792

Religiosity       <–>     Holistic                    -0.255                    ***                           -3.770

___________________________________________________________________________

Attitudes towards ADHD: The higher the religiosity (Beta = 0.148), stigmatization (Beta = 0.467), and holistic thinking (Beta = 0.197), the more negative the attitudes towards ADHD. Internal locus of control was not significantly associated with attitudes towards ADHD. Perceptions of ADHD: While high religiosity was associated with more accurate/favorable perceptions of ADHD (Beta = -0.103), internal locus of control (Beta = 0.170), and holistic thinking (Beta = 0.599) were associated with more inaccurate/unfavorable perceptions of ADHD. Stigmatization was not significantly associated with perceptions of ADHD. 

Unlike the other cultural factors in which higher scores resulted in more negative perceptions towards ADHD, the inverse was true for religiosity. The researcher hypothesized that higher religiosity would result in more inaccurate/unfavorable perceptions towards ADHD. However, in this instance those higher in religiosity had more favorable perceptions towards ADHD. Nevertheless, higher religiosity was associated with negative attitudes towards ADHD, which was in line with what the researcher hypothesized.

The researcher believed that high religiosity would be associated with both negative perceptions and negative attitudes towards ADHD; however, the results were mixed. Perhaps, these results indicate that those high in religiosity may perceive ADHD accurately, while still having the tendency to stigmatize ADHD. Based on the operationalized definition of perceptions of ADHD and attitudes towards ADHD for the purposes of this study, it may be possible to have both accurate/favorable perceptions of ADHD, while still having negative attitudes towards the ADHD. 

Figure 3

Six-factor Structural Model Representing the Latent Constructs

Discussion. Previous research has indicated a potential link to these cultural factors and negative opinions of ADHD and/or mental health disorders. For example, Li (2008) found a link between religiosity and negative opinions of ADHD, Wong-Anuchit, et al. (2016) reported on Thai cultural tendencies to stigmatize mental disorders, Kwan and Chiu (2014) found a connection between holistic thinking and the potential tendency to inaccurately attribute blame, and Beckman (1972) proposed that those with low internal locus of control may be more likely to stigmatize mental disorders. In order to investigate the relationship between these cultural factors and perceptions/attitudes towards ADHD, path models were examined.

The current research found a link between cultural factors prominent in Thailand and negative perceptions/attitudes towards ADHD. Religiosity, stigmatization, and holistic thinking were associated with negative attitudes towards ADHD, while internal locus of control and holistic thinking were associated with negative perceptions of ADHD. Religiosity was associated with favorable/accurate perceptions of ADHD.

Religiosity appeared to be the most inconsistent cultural factor. It was associated with negative attitudes towards ADHD, but favorable/accurate perceptions of ADHD. Since high religiosity was associated with negative attitudes towards ADHD, it would be expected that it would also be associated with unfavorable/inaccurate perceptions; however, the inverse resulted with high religiosity being associated with favorable perceptions of ADHD. Those high in religiosity may tend to stigmatize ADHD, while also having favorable/accurate perceptions of the disorder.

Only holistic thinking resulted in both negative attitudes towards ADHD and unfavorable/inaccurate perceptions of ADHD. Perhaps the tendency to inaccurately attribute blame which results from holistic thinking leads to more negative attitudes and perceptions towards ADHD (Kwan and Chiu 2014).

Table 6

Cultural factors significantly associated with negative attitudes and perceptions towards ADHD.

Negative Attitudes                             Unfavorable/Inaccurate Perceptions ______________________________________________________________________________

                        Stigmatization                                                             Holistic Thinking

                        Religiosity                                                                   Internal Locus of Control                               Holistic Thinking       

Overall, all cultural factors investigated in the current study were associated with negative attitudes towards ADHD or inaccurate/unfavorable perceptions of ADHD in at least one instance. This is significant, as research indicates that these cultural factors are more prevalent in Thai culture than in Western culture, which may contribute to less favorable opinions on ADHD.

High scores on holistic thinking were associated with both negative attitudes and unfavorable/inaccurate perceptions of ADHD, while the other cultural factors were only associated either negative attitudes or negative perceptions (not both). This suggests that this cultural factor may be result in more overall negative opinions of ADHD. However, on the other hand, high religiosity was also associated with favorable/accurate perceptions of ADHD and negative attitudes towards ADHD. Perhaps aspects of religiosity in Thailand serves to increase negative attitudes towards ADHD, while at the same time resulting in favorable and or accurate perceptions of the disorder. This could be investigated further in future research.

Limitations of the Study. The study should be interpreted cautiously before generalizing the results. Firstly, all the questionnaires were translated from their original language (English) to Thai. Although it was determined that the translation was accurate, it is always possible that some subtleties of language are lost in translation.

The results of the study only represent relationships between variables. Therefore, regarding the results, causality cannot be attributed between independent and dependent variables. The observed significant path coefficients only signify these relationships.

Implications of the Findings. The current study revealed that cultural factors prominent in Thailand were associated with negative attitudes and unfavorable/inaccurate perceptions of ADHD. Particularly, stigmatization of mental illness and holistic thinking were consistently associated with negative opinions towards ADHD, while health locus of control and religiosity were somewhat less consistently associated with negative opinions towards ADHD.

Furthermore, previous exposure to ADHD (knowing someone with ADHD) lessened the association between these cultural factors and negative opinions towards ADHD, which shows that familiarity with the disorder reduces negative opinions towards ADHD. This further highlights the importance of educating Thais about ADHD, since building familiarity with ADHD may even override cultural influences—serving to mitigate negative opinions towards ADHD.

Avenues for Future Research. (1) Teaching tools/methods should be researched and developed to reduce stigmatization of ADHD and other mental health disorders. Such educational tools/methods could be implemented in schools with students and teachers, as well as with parents to reduce the stigma and shame associated with mental health disorders. This would decrease resistance towards an ADHD diagnosis and increase treatment-seeking behaviors. (2) Future research could investigate in more detail the results of this study that linked religiosity with both positive perceptions of ADHD and negative attitudes towards ADHD, simultaneously. 

Conclusion. The present study investigated the relationship between cultural factors and attitudes/perceptions of ADHD in Thailand. This research contribute to understanding the relationship between these cultural factors and ADHD. Therefore, the present study contributed to expanding the knowledge of ADHD in Thailand.The current research found that there was support for the hypotheses that this researcher proposed, although further research on this topic is needed. For example, the current research suggested that there was a relationship between Thai cultural factors and negative opinions towards ADHD.

In addition, the current research found that cultural factors prevalent in Thailand were associated with negative attitudes and perceptions of ADHD. Future research should focus on improving awareness and knowledge of ADHD in Thailand and reducing stigma towards the disorder. In so doing, the current research along with future research broadens the base of knowledge of ADHD in Thailand in hopes of improving access for treatment of ADHD; thus, improving the lives of children, teens, and adults with ADHD.

REFERENCES

Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103(3), 411–423.

Barkley, R. (2001). The inattentive type of adhd as a distinct disorder: What remains to be done. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 489 – 493. doi:10.1093/clipsy.8.4.489

Beckman, L. (1972). Locus of control and attitudes toward mental illness among mental health volunteers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 38(1), 84-8.

Benjasuwantep, B., Ruangdaraganon, N., & Visudhiphan, P. (2002). Prevalence and clinical characteristics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among primary school students in bangkok. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand, 85(4), 1232-40.

Cheng, C., & Cheung, S. F. (2013). Cultural meaning of perceived control: A meta-analysis of locus of control and psychological symptoms across 18 cultural regions. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 152-188.

Faraone, S., Sergeant, J., Gillberg, C., & Biederman, B. (2003). The worldwide prevalence of adhd: Is it an american condition? World Psychiatry, 2, 2.

Fuermaier, A.B., et al. (2014). Stigmatization in teachers towards adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. SpringerPlus (3),26.

Kaewprom C., Curtis J., & Deane, F.P. (2011). Factors involved in recovery from schizophrenia: A qualitative study of Thai mental health nurses. Nurs Health Sci, 13(3), 323-327.

Kwan, L., & Chiu, C. (2014). Holistic versus analytic thinking explains collective culpability attribution. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,36, 38.

Lauber, C., & Rössler, W. (2007). Stigma towards people with mental illness in developing countries in Asia. Taylor & Francis.

Li, K. (2013). Religion and medicalization: The case of adhd.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 52(2), 309–327

Nisbett, R. E., & Miyamoto, Y. (2005). The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 467-473.

Taylor, J. (2008). Buddhism and Postmodern Imaginings in Thailand. London: Routledge.

Wong-Anuchit, C., Mills A., Schneider J., Rujkorakarn, D., Kerdpongbunchote, C., & Panyayong, B. (2016). Internalized stigma of mental illness scale – thai version: Translation and assessment of psychometric properties among psychiatric outpatients in central thailand. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 30, 450–456

Reid, R., DuPaul, G., Anastopoulos, A., & Riccio, C. (1998). Assessing culturally different students for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using behavior rating scales. Journal of Child Psychology,26, 187-198.

Sakboonyarat, B., et al. (2018). Prevalence and associated factors of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (adhd) in a rural community, central thailand: A mixed methods study. Canadian Center of Science and Education, 10, 60-69.

Visanuyothin, T., et al. (2013). The prevalence of attention deficit/hyperactivitydisorder in Thailand. Journal of Mental Health of Thailand, 21(2), 394-356.

Photo by Yavor Punchev on Unsplash

This study has now been published in the Assumption University Journals, view the published version here

Book an Appointment

Contact us now to organise an appointment
CONTACT US
Lighthouse rehabilitation center is accredited and approved by the Ministry of Public health.
Address 1:
Lighthouse Human Services and Counseling Bangkok
TT Building 888, Sukhumvit 81, Phra Khanong
Bangkok, Thailand, 10260
Phone: 0922 887 234
Address 2:
The Racquet Club
Sukhumvit 49/9
menu-circle